The Science of Animal Sentience
“Though the fact of animal sentience is implicit in experimentation, researchers have traditionally downplayed and ignored certain aspects of it, and in nonvertebrate species they have often denied it altogether. While it is established that vertebrate animals feel pain and respond to pain drugs in much the same ways that humans do, emotions such as joy, happiness, suffering, empathy, and fear have often been ignored, despite the fact that many psychological and behavioral experiments are predicated on the assumption that animals feel these emotions and will consistently react based on these feelings. The evidence for animal sentience is overwhelming, yet denial of the emotional states of animals still occurs in the research community.”
—Ingrid L. Taylor, D.V.M.
To learn more, read PETA’s report “Animal Sentience and Emotions: The Argument for Universal Acceptance” and watch the webinar series “Invertebrate Animal Sentience,” part 1 and part 2, hosted by WellBeing International.
Relevant Publications
Abramson CI and Calvo P. (2022). Unresolved issues of behavioral analysis in invertebrates. Animal Sentience. 32(18). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1739.
Allen C. (2013). Fish cognition and consciousness. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics. 26(1):25–39. doi:10.1007/s10806-011-9364-9.
Allen C, Grau JW, Meagher, MW. (2009). The lower bounds of cognition: what do spinal cords reveal? In: Bickle J, ed. The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy and Neuroscience.Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press. 129–142. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195304787.003.0006.
Alupay JS, Hadjisolomou SP, and Crook RJ. (2014). Arm injury produces long‐term behavioral and neural hypersensitivity in octopus. Neuroscience Letters. 558:137–142. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2013.11.002.
Andrews, K. (2022). Does the sentience framework imply all animals are sentient? Animal Sentience. 32(17). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1737.
Andrews PLR, Darmaillacq A-S, Dennison N, Gleadall IG, Hawkins P, Messenger, JB, Osorio D, Smith VJ, and Smith JA. (2013). The identification and management of pain, suffering and distress in cephalopods, including anaesthesia, analgesia and humane killing. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology. 447:46–64. doi:10.1016/j.jembe.2013.02.010.
Appel M and Elwood RW. (2009). Motivational trade‐offs and potential pain experience in hermit crabs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 119(1–2):120–124. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2009.03.013.
Ashley PJ, Ringrose S, Edwards KL, Wallington E, McCrohan CR, and Sneddon, LU. (2009). Effect of noxious stimulation upon antipredator responses and dominance status in rainbow trout. Animal Behaviour. 77(2):403–410. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.10.015.
Auersperg AMI, von Bayern AMP, Gajdon GK, Huber L, and Kacelnik A. (2011). Flexibility in problem solving and tool use of Kea and New Caledonian crows in a multi access box paradigm. PLOS One. 6(6):e20231. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020231.
Auvray M, Myin E, and Spence, C. (2010). The sensory‐discriminative and affective‐ motivational aspects of pain. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(2):214–223. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.008.
Barr S, Laming PR, Dick JT, and Elwood, RW. (2008). Nociception or pain in a decapod crustacean? Animal Behaviour. 75(3):745–751. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.004.
Barron AB and Klein C. (2016). What insects can tell us about the origins of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 113(18):4900-4908. doi:10.1073/pnas.1520084113.
Bateson M, Desire S, Gartside SE, and Wright GA. (2011). Agitated honeybees exhibit pessimistic cognitive biases. Current Biology. 21(12):1070–1073. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.017.
Bayne T. (2018). On the axiomatic foundations of the integrated information theory of consciousness. Neuroscience of Consciousness. 4(1):niy007. doi:10.1093/nc/niy007.
Bazarini SN and Crook RJ. (2020). Environmental estrogen exposure disrupts sensory processing and nociceptive plasticity in the cephalopod Euprymna scolopes. Journal of Experimental Biology. 223:jeb218008. doi:10.1242/jeb.218008.
Bekoff M. (2000). Animal Emotions: exploring passionate natures: current interdisciplinary research provides compelling evidence that many animals experience such emotions as joy, fear, love, despair, and grief—we are not alone. BioScience. 50(10):861–870. doi:10.1641/0006-3568(2000)050[0861:AEEPN]2.0.CO;2.
Ben‐Haim MS, Dal Monte O, Fagan NA, Dunham Y, Hassin RR, Chang SW, and Santos LR. (2021). Disentangling perceptual awareness from nonconscious processing in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 118(15):e2017543118. doi:10.1073/pnas.2017543118.
Bentham, J. (1789). An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation. Anta CG, ed. Rome: Aracne Editrice, 2019.
Berridge K. and Winkielman P. (2003). What is an unconscious emotion? (The case for unconscious “liking”). Cognition and Emotion. 17(2):181–211. doi:10.1080/02699930302289.
Birch J. (2018). Animal cognition and human values. Philosophy of Science. 85(5):1026–1037. doi:10.1086/699744.
Birch J. (2017). Animal sentience and the precautionary principle. (2017). Animal Sentience: An Interdisciplinary Journal on Animal Feeling. 16(1). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1200.
Birch J. (2020a). Global workspace theory and animal consciousness. Philosophical Topics. 48(1):21–37. doi:10.5840/philtopics20204812.
Birch J. (2020b). The search for invertebrate consciousness. Noûs. doi:10.1111/nous.12351.
Birch J, Ginsburg S, and Jablonka E. (2020). Unlimited Associative Learning and the origins of consciousness: a primer and some predictions. Biology and Philosophy. 35,56. doi:10.1007/s10539-020-09772-0.
Birch J, Schnell AK, and Clayton NS. (2020). Dimensions of animal consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 24(10):789–801. dob:10.1016/j.tics.2020.07.007.
Black D. (2020). The global workspace theory, the phenomenal concept strategy, and the distribution of consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition. 84(5),102992. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2020.102992.
Block NJ. (1995). On a confusion about the function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 18(2):227–247. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00038188.
Boly M, Seth AK, Wilke M, Ingmundson P, Baars B, Laureys S, Edelman D, and Tsuchiya N. (2013). Consciousness in humans and non‐human animals: recent advances and future directions. Frontiers in Psychology. 4,625. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00625.
Bradshaw RH. (1998). Consciousness in non‐human animals: adopting the precautionary principle. Journal of Consciousness Studies. 5:108–114. doi:10.1177/02611929990270050.
Braithwaite VA. Do Fish Feel Pain? (2010). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Briefer EF, Haque S, Baciadonna L, McElligott AG. Goats excel at learning and remembering a highly novel cognitive task. (2014). Frontiers in Zoology. 11(1):20. doi:10.1186/1742-9994-11-20.
Briefer EF, Padilla de la Torre M, McElligott AG. (2012). Mother goats do not forget their kids’ calls. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 279(1743):3749–3755. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.0986.
Briefer EF and McElligott AG. (2013). Rescued goats at a sanctuary display positive mood after former neglect. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 146(1):45–55. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2013.03.007.
Briffa M. (2022). Sentience in decapods: an open question. Animal Sentience. 32(19). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1740.
Bronfman ZZ, Ginsburg S, and Jablonka E. (2016). The transition to minimal consciousness through the evolution of associative learning. Frontiers in Psychology. 7,1954. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01954.
Broom DM. (2014). Sentience and animal welfare. Wallingford, Oxfordshire: CABI.
Broom DM. (2016). Considering animals’ feelings. Animal Sentience. 5(1). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1015.
Brown C. (2015). Fish intelligence, sentience and ethics. Animal Cognition. 18(1):1–17. doi:10.1007/s10071-014-0761-0.
Brown C. (2017). A risk assessment and phylogenetic approach. Animal Sentience. 16(3). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1219.
Brown SAB. (2022) How much of a pain would a crustacean “common currency” really be? Animal Sentience. 32(23). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1749.
Browning H. (2020). If I could talk to the animals: measuring subjective animal welfare. Ph.D. dissertation. Australian National University. doi:10.25911/5f1572fb1b5be.
Browning H and Veit W. (2020). The measurement problem of consciousness. Philosophical Topics. 48(1):85–108. doi:10.5840/philtopics20204815.
Butlin P. (2022). Sentience criteria to persuade the reasonable sceptic. Animal Sentience. 32(21). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1741.
Carruthers P. (2018a). Comparative psychology without consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition. 63, 47–60. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2018.06.012.
Carruthers P. (2018b). The problem of animal consciousness. Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association. 92:179–205. https://faculty.philosophy.umd.edu/pcarruthers/The%20problem%20of%20animal%20consciousness.pdf. Accessed February 29, 2024.
Carruthers P. (2019). Human and Animal Minds: The Consciousness Questions Laid to Rest. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Carruthers P. (2020). Stop caring about consciousness. Philosophical Topics. 48(1):1–20. doi:10.5840/philtopics20204811.
Cheng F. (2017). Can animals be treated well in laboratories? The Ark. 236:35-37.
Chittka L and Niven J. (2009). Are bigger brains better? Current Biology. 19(21):R995–R1008. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.023. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.023.
Clark RE and Squire LR. (1998). Classical conditioning and brain systems: the role of awareness. Science. 280(5360):77–81. doi:10.1126/science.280.5360.77.
Clark RE, Manns JR, and Squire LH. (2002). Classical conditioning, awareness, and brain systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 12:524–531. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(02)02041-7.
Cooper JJ, Tinarwo A, and Ventura BA. (2022). Decapods as food, companions and research animals: legal impact of ascribing sentience. Animal Sentience. 32(27). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1759.
Crook RJ. (2021). Behavioral and neurophysiological evidence suggests affective pain experience in octopus. iScience. 24(3), 102229. doi:10.1016/j.isci.2021.102229.
Crook RJ, Dickson K, Hanlon RT, and Walters ET. (2014). Nociceptive sensitization reduces predation risk. Current Biology. 24(10):1121–1125. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.043.
Crook RJ, Hanlon RT, and Walters ET. (2013). Squid have nociceptors that display widespread long‐term sensitization and spontaneous activity after bodily injury. Journal of Neuroscience. 33(24):10021–10026. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0646-13.2013.
Crook RJ, Lewis T, Hanlon RT, and Walters ET. (2011). Peripheral injury induces long‐term sensitization of defensive responses to visual and tactile stimuli in the squid Loligo pealeii. Journal of Experimental Biology. 214(19):3171–3185. doi:10.1242/jeb.058131
Crump A, Browning H, Schnell A, Burn C, and Birch J. (2022). Animal sentience research: Synthesis and proposals. Animal Sentience. 32(31). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1770.
Da Costa AP, Leigh AE, Man MS, and Kendrick KM. (2004) Face pictures reduce behavioural, autonomic, endocrine and neural indices of stress and fear in sheep. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 271(1552):2077-2084. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2831.
Dawkins MS. (2012). Why Animals Matter: Animal Consciousness, Animal Welfare and Human Well‐Being. New York: Oxford University Press.
Dawkins MS. (2017). Animal welfare with and without consciousness. Journal of Zoology. 301(1):1–10. doi:10.1111/jzo.12434.
Dawkins MS. (2021). The Science of Animal Welfare: Understanding What Animals Want. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dawkins MS. (2022). Unconscious humans, autonomous machines and the difficulty of knowing which animals are sentient. Animal Sentience. 32(20). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1746.
DeGrazia D. (1996). Taking Animals Seriously: Mental Life and Moral Status. Cambridge University Press.
de Waal FBM. (2022). Sentience as part of emotional lives. Animal Sentience. 32(22). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1747.
de Waal FBM. What is an animal emotion? (2011). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1224(1):191–206. https://www.emory.edu/LIVING_LINKS/publications/articles/deWaal_2011b.pdf. Accessed February 29, 2024.
Dehaene S. (2014). Consciousness and the Brain: Deciphering How the Brain Encodes Our Thoughts. New York: Viking Press.
Dehaene S and Changeux J‐P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron. 70(2):200–227. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018.
Dehaene S, Changeux J-P, and Naccache L. (2011). The global neuronal workspace model of conscious access: from neuronal architectures to clinical applications. In: Dehaene S and Christen Y, eds. Characterizing Consciousness: From Cognition to the Clinic? Berlin: Springer. 55–84.
Diarte‐Plata G, Sainz‐Hernández JC, Aguinãga‐Cruz JA, Fierro‐Coronado JA, Polanco‐Torres A, and Puente‐Palazuelos C. (2012). Eyestalk ablation procedures to minimize pain in the freshwater prawn Macrobrachium americanum. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 140(3–4):172–178. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2012.06.002.
Diggles BK. (2019). Review of some scientific issues related to crustacean welfare. ICES Journal of Marine Science. 76(1):66–81. doi:10.1093/icesjms/fsy058.
Dinets V. (2016). No cortex, no cry. Animal Sentience. 1(3),7. doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1027.
Douglas HE. (2009). Science, Policy and the Value‐Free Ideal. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.
Droege P, Weiss DJ, Schwob N, and Braithwaite V. (2021). Trace conditioning as a test for animal consciousness: a new approach. Animal Cognition. 8. doi:10.3389/fvets.2021.785256.
Duncan IJ. (2002). Poultry welfare: science or subjectivity? British Poultry Science. 43(5):643–652. doi:10.1080/0007166021000025109.
Duncan IJ. (2006). The changing concept of animal sentience. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 100(1–2):11–19. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2006.04.011.
Dyuizen IV, Kotsyuba EP, and Lamash NE. (2012). Changes in the nitric oxide system in the shore crab Hemigrapsus sanguineus (Crustacea, decapoda) CNS induced by a nociceptive stimulus. Journal of Experimental Biology. 215(15):2668–2676. doi:10.1242/jeb.066845.
Edgar J, Held S, Paul E, Pettersson I, I’Anson Price R, and Nicol C. (2015). Social buffering in a bird. Animal Behaviour. 105:11–19. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.04.007.
Edgar JL, Paul ES, and Nicol CJ. (2013). Protective mother hens: cognitive influence on the avian maternal response. Animal Behaviour. 86(2):223–229. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.05.004.
Eisemann CH, Jorgensen WK, Merritt DJ, Rice MJ, Cribb BW, Webb PD, and Zalucki MP. (1984). Do insects feel pain?—A biological view. Experientia. 40(2):164–167. doi:10.1007/bf01963580.
Elwood RW and Appel M. (2009) Pain experience in hermit crabs? Animal Behaviour. 77(5):1243–1246. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.028.
Elwood RW, Barr S, and Patterson L. (2009) Pain and stress in crustaceans? Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 118(3–4):128–136. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.018.
Emery NJ and Clayton NS. (2015). Do birds have the capacity for fun? Current Biology. 25(1):R16–R20. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.09.020.
Feinberg TE and Mallatt J. (2019). Subjectivity “demystified”: neurobiology, evolution, and the explanatory gap. Frontiers in Psychology. 10,1686. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01686.
Feinberg TE and Mallatt JM. (2016). The Ancient Origins of Consciousness: How the Brain Created Experience. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Feinberg TE and Mallatt JM. (2018). Consciousness Demystified. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Finlayson K, Lampe JF, Hintze S, Würbel H, and Melotti L. (2016). Facial indicators of positive emotions in rats. PLOS One. 11(11):e0166446. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0166446.
Finn JK, Tregenza T, and Norman MD. (2009). Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus. Current Biology. 19(23):R1069-R1070. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.10.052.
Fossat P, Bacqué‐Cazenave J, De Deurwaerdère P, Cattaert D, and Delbecque, JP. (2015). Serotonin, but not dopamine, controls the stress response and anxiety‐like behavior in the crayfish Procambarus clarkii. Journal of Experimental Biology. 218(Pt 17):2745–2752. doi:10.1242/jeb.120550.
Fossat P, Bacqué‐Cazenave J, De Deurwaerdère P, Delbecque JP, and Cattaert D. (2014). Anxiety‐like behavior in crayfish is controlled by serotonin. Science. 344(6189):1293–1297. doi:10.1126/science.1248811.
Frank MG, Waldrop RH, Dumoulin M, Aton S, and Boal JG. (2012). A preliminary analysis of sleep-like states in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. PLOS One. 7(6):e38125. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0038125.
Gallup AC, Swartwood L, Militello J, Sackett S. (2015). Experimental evidence of contagious yawning in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Animal Cognition. 18(5):1051–1058. doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0873-1.
Gibbons M and Chittka L. (2022). A framework for evaluating evidence of pain in animals. Animal Sentience. 32(28). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1767.
Gibbons M and Sarlak S. (2020). Inhibition of pain or response to injury in invertebrates and vertebrates. Animal Sentience. 29(34). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1649.
Ginsburg S and Jablonka E. (2019). The Evolution of the Sensitive Soul: Learning and the Origins of Consciousness. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Godfrey-Smith P. (2017). The mind of an octopus: eight smart limbs plus a big brain add up to a weird and wondrous kind of intelligence. Scientific American. https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/the-mind-of-an-octopus/. Accessed February 29, 2024.
Godfrey‐Smith P. (2016). Other Minds: The Octopus, the Sea, and the Deep Origins of Consciousness. New York: Farrar, Strauss and Giroux.
Goodwin GP. (2015). Experimental approaches to moral standing. Philosophy Compass. 10(12):914–926. doi:10.1111/phc3.12266.
Groening J, Venini D, and Srinivasan M. (2017). In search of evidence for the experience of pain in honeybees: a self‐administration study. Scientific Reports. 7(1),45825. doi:10.1038/srep45825.
Güntürkün O and Bugnyar T. (2016). Cognition without cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 20(4):291–303. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.02.001.
Hamilton TJ, Kwan GT, Gallup J, and Tresguerres M. (2016). Acute fluoxetine exposure alters crab anxiety‐like behaviour, but not aggressiveness. Scientific Reports. 6(1),19850. doi:10.1038/srep19850.
Harnad S. (2016). Animal sentience: the other minds problem. Animal Sentience. 1(1). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1065.
Horback K. (2014). Nosing around: play in pigs. Animal Behavior and Cognition. 1(2):186–196. doi:10.12966/abc.05.08.2014.
Howard RB, Lopes LN, Lardie CR, Perez PP, and Crook RJ. (2019). Early‐life injury produces lifelong neural hyperexcitability, cognitive deficit and altered defensive behaviour in the squid Euprymna scolopes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 374(1785), 20190281. doi:10.1098/rstb.2019.0281.
Iglesias TL, McElreath R, and Patricelli GL. (2012) Western scrub-jay funerals: cacophonous aggregations in response to dead conspecifics. Animal Behaviour. 84(5):1103–1111. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.08.007.
Jack AI and Robbin P. (2012). The phenomenal stance revisited. Review of Philosophy and Psychology. 3:383–403. doi:10.1007/s13164-012-0104-5.
Jones RC (2013). Science, sentience, and animal welfare. Biology and Philosophy. 28:1–30. doi:10.1007/s10539-012-9351-1.
Kakrada E and Colombo M. (2022). Extending the null hypothesis to invertebrate pain sentience. Animal Sentience. 32(25). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1754.
Key B. (2014). Fish do not feel pain and its implications for understanding phenomenal consciousness. Biology & Philosophy. 30(2):149–165. doi:10.1007/s10539-014-9469-4.
Key B. (2016). Why fish do not feel pain. Animal Sentience. 3(1). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1011.
Key B and Brown D. (2022). Lack of imagination can bias our view of animal sentience. Animal Sentience. 32(26). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1756.
Kirkwood JK. (2006). The distribution of the capacity for sentience in the animal kingdom. In: Turner J and D’Silva J. eds. Animals, Ethics and Trade: The Challenge of Animal Sentience. London: Earthscan. 12–26.
Klein C and Barron AB. (2016). Insects have the capacity for subjective experience. Animal Sentience. 9(1). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1113.
Knolle F, Goncalves RP, and Morton AJ. (2017). Sheep recognize familiar and unfamiliar human faces from two-dimensional images. Royal Society Open Science. 4(11):171228. doi:10.1098/rsos.171228.
Knutsson S and Munthe C. (2017). A virtue of precaution regarding the moral status of animals with uncertain sentience. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics. 30(2):213–224. doi:10.1007/s10806-017-9662-y.
Kuba MJ, Byrne RA, Meisel DV, and Mather JA. (2006). When do octopuses play? Effects of repeated testing, object type, age, and food deprivation on object play in Octopus vulgaris. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 120(3):184–190. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.120.3.184.
Langford DJ, Crager SE, Shehzad Z, Smith SB, Sotocinal SG, Levenstadt JS, Chanda ML, Levitin DJ, and Mogil JS. (2006). Social modulation of pain as evidence for empathy in mice. Science. 312(5782):1967–1970. doi:10.1126/science.1128322.
LeDoux J. (2019). The Deep History of Ourselves: The Four‐Billion‐Year Story of How We Got Conscious Brains. New York: Viking Press.
Levin M. (2022). Generalizing frameworks for sentience beyond natural species. Animal Sentience. 32(15). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1733.
Low P, Panksepp J, Reiss D, Edelman D, Van Swinderen B, and Koch C. (2012) The Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness. https://fcmconference.org/img/CambridgeDeclarationOnConsciousness.pdf. Accessed February 29, 2024.
Makowska IJ and Weary DM. (2013). Assessing the emotions of laboratory rats. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 148(1–2):1–12. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2013.07.017.
Mallatt J. (2017). Shoring up the precautionary BAR. Animal Sentience. 16(7). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1229.
Mallatt J and Feinberg TE. (2022). Decapod sentience: promising framework and evidence. Animal Sentience. 32(24). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1750.
Marino L. (2017). Thinking chickens: a review of cognition, emotion, and behavior in the domestic chicken. Animal Cognition. 20:127–147. doi:10.1007/s10071-016-1064-4.
Marino L and Allen K. (2017). The psychology of cows. Animal Behavior and Cognition. 4(4):474–498. doi:10.26451/abc.04.04.06.2017.
Marino L and Colvin CM. (2015). Thinking pigs: a comparative review of cognition, emotion, and personality in Sus domesticus. International Journal of Comparative Psychology. 28:23859. doi:10.46867/ijcp.2015.28.00.04.
Mashour GA, Roelfsema P, Changeux, J-P, and Dehaene S. (2020). Conscious processing and the global neuronal workspace hypothesis. Neuron. 105(5):776–798. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.026.
Mather JA. (2008). Cephalopod consciousness: behavioural evidence. Consciousness and Cognition. 17(1):37–48. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2006.11.006.
Mather JA and Anderson RC. (1999). Exploration, play and habituation in octopuses (Octopus dofleini). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 113(3):333–338. doi:doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.113.3.333.
McCambridge C, Dick JT, and Elwood RW. (2016). Effects of autotomy compared to manual declawing on contests between males for females in the edible crab Cancer pagurus: implications for fishery practice and animal welfare. Journal of Shellfish Research. 35(4):1037–1044. doi:10.2983/035.035.0426.
Mellor DJ, Beausoleil NJ, Littlewood KE, McLean AN, McGreevy PD, Jones B, and Wilkins C. (2020). The 2020 Five Domains model: including human-animal interactions in assessments of animal welfare. Animals. 10(10):1870. doi:10.3390/ani10101870.
Mendl M, Burman OHP, Parker RMA, and Paul ES. (2009). Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 118(3–4):161–181. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.023.
Merker BH. (2007). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: a challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 30(1):63–81. doi:10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00002-3.
Michel M. (2019). Fish and microchips: on fish pain and multiple realization. Philosophical Studies. 25(9):95–110. doi:10.1007/s11098-018-1133-4.
Millot S, Cerqueira M, Castanheira MF, Øverli Ø, Martins CIM, and Oliveira, RF. (2014). Use of conditioned place preference/avoidance tests to assess affective states in fish. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 154:104–111. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2014.02.004.
Montemayor C. (2022). Distinguishing epistemic and moral grounds for legal protection. Animal Sentience 32(16). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1734.
Müller JJA, Massen JJM, Bugnyar T, and Osvath M. (2017). Ravens remember the nature of a single reciprocal interaction sequence over 2 days and even after a month. Animal Behaviour. 128:69–78. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.04.004.
Nagel T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review. 83(4):435–450. doi:10.2307/2183914.
Nawroth C, Brett JM, and McElligott AG. (2016). Goats display audience-dependent human-directed gazing behaviour in a problem-solving task. Biology Letters. 12(7). doi:10.1098/rsbl.2016.0283.
Nieder A, Wagener L, and Rinnert P. (2020). A neural correlate of sensory consciousness in a corvid bird. Science. 369(6511):1626–1629. doi:10.1126/science.abb1447.
Núñez J, Maldonado H, Miralto A, and Balderrama N. (1983). The stinging response of the honeybee: effects of morphine, naloxone and some opioid peptides. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior. 19(6):921–924. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(83)90391-x.
Obozova T, Smirnova A, Zorina Z, and Wasserman E. (2015). Analogical reasoning in amazons. Animal Cognition. 18(6):1363–1371. doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0882-0.
Oshima M, di Pauli von Treuheim T, Carroll J, Hanlon RT, Walters ET, and Crook RJ. (2016). Peripheral injury alters schooling behavior in squid, Doryteuthis pealeii. Behavioural Processes. 128:89–95. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2016.04.008.
Osvath M and Sima M. (2014). Sub-adult ravens synchronize their play: A case of emotional contagion? Animal Behavior and Cognition. 1(2):197–205. doi:10.12966/abc.05.09.2014.
Paul ES, Sher S, Tamietto M, Winkielman P, and Mendl MT. (2020). Towards a comparative science of emotion: affect and consciousness in humans and animals. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 108:749–770. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.11.014.
Perez PV, Butler‐Struben HM, and Crook RJ. (2017). The selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor fluoxetine increases spontaneous afferent firing, but not mechanonociceptive sensitization, in octopus. Invertebrate Neuroscience. 17(4):10. doi:10.1007/s10158-017-0203-1.
Perry CJ, Baciadonna L, and Chittka L. (2016). Unexpected rewards induce dopamine‐dependent positive emotion–like state changes in bumblebees. Science. 353(6307):1529–1531. doi:10.1126/science.aaf4454.
Pitts MA, Lutsyshyna LA, and Hillyard SA. (2018). The relationship between attention and consciousness: an expanded taxonomy and implications for “no‐report” paradigms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 373(1755), 20170348. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0348.
Powell R and Mikhalevich I. (2021). Affective sentience and moral protection. Animal Sentience. 29(35). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1668.
Proctor HS, Carder G, and Cornish AR. (2013). Searching for animal sentience: a systematic review of the scientific literature. Animals. 3(3):882–906. doi:10.3390/ani3030882.
Proctor H. (2012). Animal sentience: where are we and where are we heading? Animals. 2(4):628–639. doi:10.3390/ani2040628.
Puri S and Faulkes Z. (2010). Do decapod crustaceans have nociceptors for extreme pH? PLOS One. 5(4):e10244. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010244.
Puri S and Faulkes Z. (2015). Can crayfish take the heat? Procambarus clarkii show nociceptive behaviour to high temperature stimuli, but not low temperature or chemical stimuli. Biology Open. 4(4):441–448. doi:10.1242/bio.20149654.
Rainville P, Duncan GH, Price DD, Carrier B, and Bushnell MC. (1997). Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science. 277(5328):968–971. doi:10.1126/science.277.5328.968.
Reefmann N, Bütikofer Kaszàs F, Wechsler B, and Gygax L. (2009). Physiological expression of emotional reactions in sheep. Physiology & Behavior. 98(1–2):235–241. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.05.017.
Reimert I, Bolhuis JE, Kemp B, and Rodenburg TB. (2013). Indicators of positive and negative emotions and emotional contagion in pigs. Physiology & Behavior. 109:42–50. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.11.002.
Rink E and Wullimann MF. (2004). Connections of the ventral telencephalon (subpallium) in the zebrafish (Danio rerio). Brain Research. 1011(2):206–220. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2004.03.027.
Rose JD. (2002). The neurobehavioral nature of fishes and the question of awareness and pain. Reviews in Fisheries Science. 10(1):1–38. doi:10.1080/20026491051668.
Rose JD, Arlinghaus R, Cooke SJ, Diggles BK, Sawynok W, Stevens ED, and Wynne CD. (2014). Can fish really feel pain? Fish and Fisheries. 15(1):97–133. doi:10.1111/faf.12010.
Rygula R, Pluta H, and Popik P. (2012). Laughing rats are optimistic. PLOS One. 7(12):e51959. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0051959.
Sandeman DC, Kenning M, and Harzsch S. (2014). Adaptive trends in malacostracan brain form and function related to behavior. In: Derby C and Thiel M, eds. Crustacean Nervous Systems and Their Control of Behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 11–48.
Sato N, Tan L, Tate K, and Okada M. (2015). Rats demonstrate helping behavior toward a soaked conspecific. Animal Cognition. 18(5):1039–1047. doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0872-2. [An erratum appears on p. 1049. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0906-9.]
Schaeck M, Van den Broeck W, Hermans K, and Decostere A. (2013). Fish as research tools: alternatives to in vivo experiments. Alternatives to Laboratory Animals. 41(3):219–229. doi:10.1177/026119291304100305.
Schwitzgebel E. (2016). Phenomenal consciousness, defined and defended as innocently as I can manage. Journal of Consciousness Studies. 23(11–12):224–235.
Shea N. (2012). Methodological encounters with the phenomenal kind. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research. 84(2):307–344. doi:10.1111/j.1933-1592.2010.00483.x.
Shea N and Bayne T. (2010). The vegetative state and the science of consciousness. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science. 61(3):459–484. doi:10.1093/bjps/axp046.
Shriver A. (2019). The unpleasantness of pain for humans and other animals. In: Bain D, Brady M, and Corns J, eds. Philosophy of Pain: Unpleasantness, Emotion, and Deviance. Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge. 147–162.
Simon J. (2020). Review of Carruthers P. Human and Animal Minds: The Consciousness Questions Laid to Rest. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2019. In Notre Dame Philosophical Reviews. https://ndpr.nd.edu/reviews/human-and-animal-minds-the-consciousness-questions-laid-to-rest/. Accessed March 4, 2024.
Singer P. (2022). Practical Ethics. Third edition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Siviy SM and Panksepp J. (2011). In search of the neurobiological substrates for social playfulness in mammalian brains. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 35(9):1821–1830. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.006.
Siviy SM. (1998) Neurobiological substrates of play behavior: glimpses into the structure and function of mammalian playfulness. In: Bekoff M and Byers J, eds. Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative and Ecological Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 221–242.
Skora LI, Yeomans MR, Crombag HS, and Scott RB. (2021). Evidence that instrumental conditioning requires conscious awareness in humans. Cognition. 208,104546. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104546.
Smirnova A, Zorina Z, Obozova T, and Wasserman E. (2015). Crows spontaneously exhibit analogical reasoning. Current Biology. 25(2):256–260. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.063.
Sneddon LU. (2003). The evidence for pain in fish: the use of morphine as an analgesic. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 83(2):153–162. doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(03)00113-8.
Sneddon LU. (2004). Evolution of nociception in vertebrates: comparative analysis of lower vertebrates. Brain Research Reviews. 46(2):123–130. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.07.007.
Sneddon LU, Braithwaite VA, and Gentle MJ. (2003a). Do fishes have nociceptors? Evidence for the evolution of a vertebrate sensory system. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 270(1520):1115–1121. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2349.
Sneddon LU, Braithwaite VA, and Gentle MJ. (2003b). Novel object test: examining nociception and fear in the rainbow trout. The Journal of Pain. 4(8):431–440. doi:10.1067/S1526-5900(03)00717-X.
Sneddon LU, Elwood RW, Adamo SA, and Leach MC. (2014). Defining and assessing animal pain. Animal Behaviour. 97:201–212. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.09.007.
Steel D. (2015). Philosophy and the Precautionary Principle: Science, Evidence, and Environmental Policy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Swift KN and Marzluff JM. (2015). Wild American crows gather around their dead to learn about danger. Animal Behaviour. 109:187–197. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.08.021.
Sytsma J. (2014). Attributions of consciousness. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 5(6):635–648. doi:10.1002/wcs.1320.
Sytsma J and Machery E. (2010). Two conceptions of subjective experience. Philosophical Studies. 151(2):299–327. doi:10.1007/s11098-009-9439-x.
Sytsma J and Machery E. (2012). The two sources of moral standing. Review of Philosophy and Psychology. 3(3):303–324. doi:10.1007/s13164-012-0102-7.
Szyszka P, Demmler C, Oemisch M, Sommer L, Biergans S, Birnbach B, Silbering AF, and Galizia CG. (2011). Mind the gap: olfactory trace conditioning in honeybees. Journal of Neuroscience. 31(20):7229–7239. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.6668-10.2011.
Travers E, Frith CD, and Shea N. (2018). Learning rapidly about the relevance of visual cues requires conscious awareness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 71(8):1698–1713. doi:10.1080/17470218.2017.1373834.
Tsuchiya N, Wilke M, Frässle S, and Lamme VA. (2015). No‐report paradigms: extracting the true neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 19(12):757–770. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.002.
Tye M. (2017). Tense Bees and Shell‐Shocked Crabs: Are Animals Conscious? New York: Oxford University Press.
Varner GE. (2012). Personhood, Ethics, and Animal Cognition: Situating Animals in Hare’s Two Level Utilitarianism. New York: Oxford University Press.
Veissier I, Boissy A, Désiré L, and Greiveldinger L. (2009). Animals’ emotions: studies in sheep using appraisal theories. Animal Welfare. 18(4):347–354. doi:10.1017/S0962728600000749.
Veit W. (2022). Integrating evolution into the study of animal sentience. Animal Sentience. 32(30). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1765.
Vila Pouca C and Brown C. (2017). Contemporary topics in fish cognition and behaviour. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 16:46–52. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.03.002.
Walters ET. (2022). Strong inferences about pain in invertebrates require stronger evidence. Animal Sentience. 32(14). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1731.
Watkins S and Rees G. (2007). The human superior colliculus: neither necessary, nor sufficient for consciousness? Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 30(1), 108. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0700115X.
Wimpenny JH, Weir AAS, Clayton L, Rutz C, and Kacelnik A. (2009). Cognitive processes associated with sequential tool use in New Caledonian crows. PLOS One.4(8):e6471. doi:doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006471.
Wondrak M, Conzelmann E, Veit A, and Huber L. (2018). Pigs (Sus scrofa domesticus) categorize pictures of human heads. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 205:19–27. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2018.05.009.
Woodruff ML. (2017). Scientific uncertainty and the animal sentience precautionary principle. Animal Sentience. 16(11). doi:10.51291/2377-7478.1237.
Yang TI and Chiao CC. (2016). Number sense and state-dependent valuation in cuttlefish. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 283(1837), 20161379. doi:https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2016.1379.
Zabala NA and Gómez MA. (1991). Morphine analgesia, tolerance and addiction in the cricket Pteronemobius sp.(Orthoptera, Insecta). Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 40(4):887–891. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(91)90102-8.
Zabala NA, Miralto A, Maldonado H, Nuñez JA, Jaffe K, and Calderon LDC. (1984). Opiate receptor in praying mantis: effect of morphine and naloxone. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 20(5):683–687. doi: 0.1016/0091-3057(84)90185-0.